Introduction
This chapter looks at second
language acquisition in a classroom setting. It considers whether instruction
makes a difference to SLA. This is an important issue, because it addresses the
question of the role played by environmental factors in SLA. It is also an
important educational issue, as language pedagogy has traditionally operated on
the assumption that grammar can be taught.
Two board types of SLA can be
identified according to the acquisition setting;
It was pointed out that classroom
discourse can be distorted, in comparisons to naturally occurring discourse. An
important question therefore is in what ways this distortion, which is largely
brought about by the attempt to instruct rather than to converse, affects the
route and rate of SLA in the classroom. By considering how formal instruction
affects SLA it is possible to address the wider issue of the role environmental
factors.
In many instructional methods an
assumption is made that focusing on linguistic form aids the acquisition or
grammatical knowledge or to put it another way, that raising the learner’s
consciousness about the nature of target language rules helps the learner to
internalize them.
In the case of deductive methods
this is self-evidently the case. But it is also true in ‘habbit methods’ methods
such as audio-legal lingualism , as the purpose of the practice provided is to
focus on specific linguistic forms, which the learner is encouraged to induced
and of which ultimately he will form a more or less, conscious mental
representation.
Another assumption of formal
instruction is that the order in which grammatical features are taught will
govern the order in which they are learnt. Language syllabuses are organized in
such a way as to facilitate the correlation between the teaching order and the
learning order.
The investigation of the role of
formal instruction can be undertaken in two ways. First, an answer to the
question ‘Does formal instruction aid SLA?’ can be sought. Secondly, the
question ‘What kinds of formal instruction facilitate SLA the most?’ can be
tackled. In the first question there is an assumption that all types of formal
instructions share certain basic premises and that it is, therefore possible to
talk generically of ‘ Formal instruction’. In the second question there is an assumption
that formal instruction in general is facilitative and that the important issue
is what is distinguishes the more successful from the least successful types.
What the different instructional
methods had in common was a focus on form, manifested, for instance, in the
provision of feedback by the teacher for correcting formal errors.
This chapter has four sections. The
first examines its effect on the route of SLA. The secondly examine its effect
on the rate/success of SLA. In the third section, explanations of the result
reported in the first two sections will be reviewed. Finally, the conclusion
briefly considers the implications for both SLA theory and language pedagogy.
The effects of formal instruction on
the route of SLA
The route of SLA was considered in
term of general sequence of development and the order in which specific
grammatical features were acquired. The evidence for the reported universality
of the sequence and the minor differences in the order come from (1) morpheme
studied (2) longitudinal studies. These studies how ever were of either pure
naturalistic SLA or mixed SLA. The morpheme and longitudinal studies will again
be considerers separately.
Morpheme studies of classroom SLA
The morpheme studies can be divided
into two groups. In the first group are five studies that investigated second
language learners. In the other group are four studies investigated foreign
language learners
Perkins and Larsen freeman (1975)
investigated the morpheme; they used two tasks to collect data;
- A translation test.
- A description task based on a non-dialogue film.
On (1) the morpheme orders before
and after instruction differed significantly, but on (2) there was no
significant difference. In other words, the teaching and learning orders were
different. Taken together, these studies suggest but do not prove that formal
instruction does not alter the order of acquisition of grammatical morphemes
when the learners is engaged in language use and is focused on meaning.
This general conclusion holds true
irrespective of whether the learners are children or adults and most
interestingly, irrespective of whether the learners are in foreign or second
language environments. Formal instruction appears, then to have only a
negligible effect on the morpheme order manifest in spontaneous language use.
However, morpheme orders measure accuracy rather than acquisition. In order to
obtain a more reliable picture of the effects of instruction on L2 development,
it is necessary to turn to the longitudinal studies of translation structures.
Longitudinal studies of classroom
CLA
Curiously the case-study approach,
so central to the methodological baggage of first and second language
acquisition researchers has not typically, been thought sensible for learners
in class. There are very few longitudinal studies of classroom SLA. The three
that will be discussed here are Felix (1981), Ellis (1984a) and Schumann
(1978b). The available longitudinal evidence, therefore is even slighter that
provided by the morpheme studies. The general teaching method was a traditional
audio-lingual one, the grammatical structures that Felix report on are
negation, interrogation, sentence types, and pronoun. For each structure,
parallels were found between tutored and naturalistic SLA. In a classroom where
the instruction is very formal, learners are constantly being forced to produce
structures they are not ready for. Felix suggests that they solve the problem
that this poses for them in one of two ways. Either they select random from the
structures in their repertoire, irrespective of syntactic or semantic
appropriateness, or they follow the same rules that characterize the early
stages of naturalistic language acquisition.
Ellis examined negatives,
interrogatives, and a number of verb phrase morpheme. All of these structures
were formally taught at one time. When the communicative speech produced by the
learners in the classroom was analysed, it was shown to display a pattern of
development more or less identical to that observed in naturalistic SLA.
In Schumann’s study a deliberate
attempt was made to teach an adult L2 learner how to negate. This took place in
the context of a longitudinal study of what was otherwise naturalistic SLA.
Prior to the instructional experiment the learner’s negative utterances were
collected, Schumann concluded that the instruction influenced the learner’s
production only in test-like situations, while normal communication remained
unaffected.
Taking these studies together, the
following can be hypothesized:
- Instruction does not circumvent the processes responsible for the sequence of development evident in transitional structures such as negatives an interrogative in naturalistic SLA.
- When classroom learners are required to produce structures beyond their competence, idiosyncratic forms are likely to result.
- The distorted input may prolong certain stages of development and slow down the emergence of some grammatical features.
- Classroom learners are able to make use of knowledge acquired through formal instruction when they are focused on form.
Summary
Morpheme studies and longitudinal
studies of SLA together indicate that although formal instruction may develop
L2 knowledge, this knowledge manifests itself in language use only where the
learner is attending to form. It does not, therefore, except in relatively
minor ways, affect the natural route of SLA which is evident in communicative
speech. To use the distinction between sequence and order of development, we
can say that the overall sequence of development is not affected by formal
instruction, while order of development is hardly disturbed either. Formal
instruction influences knowledge only at the careful end of the interlanguage
stylistic continuum, not the vernacular end. These conclusions, however, are
necessarily tentative, as there have been few studies of classroom SLA,
particularly longitudinal.
This section has examined three
theoretical positions which provide explanations of why formal instruction does
not affect the natural sequence of SLA but does facilitate more rapid
development. The non interface position proposed by Krashen claims that
‘acquisition’ and ‘learning are separate. Because ‘acquisition’ is responsible
for the natural sequence, the ‘learning’ that results from formal instruction
cannot influence it. However, classroom provides opportunities for
comperhesinble input will accelerate ‘acquisition’.
The interface position also posits
two types of L2 knowledge, but Argus that they are related so that ‘learning’
(or explicit knowledge) can become ‘acquisition’ (or implicit knowledge) when
it is sufficiently practised. A weaker version of this position, however,
states that ‘learning’ does not so much turn into ‘acquisition’ as facilitate
it, when the learner is ‘ready’. The variability position differs from the
other two positions in that it recognizes a variety of different ‘styles’, each
calling on knowledge types that vary in terms of analicity and automaticity.
Different task require the utilization of different kinds of knowledge. Formal
instruction contributes directly or indirectly to there internalization of
these different knowledge types and in so doing enables the classroom learner
to perform a wider range of linguistic tasks than the naturalistic learner.
All there positions provide
arguments to account for the result of the empirical research into the effect
of formal instruction. These have been considered in some detail. At the moment
there is insufficient evidence to make a clear choice between them. It is not
likely that such evidence will be forthcoming until there are more qualitative
studies of the classroom discourse that result from formal instruction and of
the linguistic development that such discourse induces.
Conclusion: some implications
This chapter began by asserting that
the investigation of the role played by instruction in SLA was of significant
for both SLA theory and language pedagogy. In this conclusion I shall briefly
consider some of the implications.
In order investigate the role of
instruction in SLA, it is necessary to separate out the effects that formal
instruction has on the route of SLA and on the rate/ successes of SLA. Where
the route is concerned, formal instruction appears to have no major effect. The
overall sequence of development associated with natural communicative language
use does not change, while only a few minor and temporary differences in the
acquisition of specific grammatical features have been observed. Thus classroom
SLA appears to involve the same processing strategies as naturalistic SLA.
Where the rate/ successes is concerned, instruction is facilitative, although
only in terms of relative utility, not in terms of absolute effects. These
results must be treated tentatively, as there has been little empirical
research.
There different positions have been
advanced to explain classroom SLA. The non interface position, associated with
Krashen (1982), distinguishes ‘acquired’ and ‘learnt’ knowledge and argues that
they are separate. This position offers a convincing explanation a why formal
instruction fails to influence the natural route of SLA, as this is a
reflection of ‘acquisition’. The explanation it gives for why formal
instruction aids the rate/ successes of SLA is less clear. The interface
position, associated with Stevick (1980) and Sharwood-Smith (1981) among
others, claim that ’learnt’ or explicit knowledge can turn into ‘acquired’ or
implicit knowledge if there is enough practice. This position offers an
explanation for the rate/ successes finding, but it less convincing about the
route finding. The variability position, associated with Tarone (1983) and
Bialystok (1982), sees acquisition and language use closely linked, such that
different types of knowledge arise from and are required for the performance of
different language tasks.
This position deals comfortably with
the route finding (which occurs in a particular kind performance) and can
explain the rate/ success finding if it is assumed that the learner who has
access to a variety of different knowledge types will outperform one who is
more reliant on a single kind of knowledge. However, it is premature to choose
from among these positions.
The study of the role of instruction
in SLA has implications for both SLA theory and language pedagogy. In the case
of the former, is stresses in importance of act knowledge the structural
properties of SLA which are relatively immune to environmental differences.
Where language pedagogy is concerned, it sheds light on the code-communications
dilemma, although once again it would be premature to come to any firm
conclusions about the effectiveness of formal grammar teaching.
Second language acquisition theory
Studying the role of instruction can
throw on the contribution of environmental factors in SLA. The classroom environment
provides a different kind of input from a natural setting. If environmental
factors are important of SLA, it might be predicted that (1) The acquisitional
route in the two setting will be different, and (2) the rate/ success of SLA in
the two setting will also differ. The research reviewed in the earlier sections
shows that (1) does not arise, while (2) may. The failure of the classroom
setting to influence the route of SLA can be explained in two ways. First, it
might be taken to show the real determinants of SLA are learner-internal rather
than environmental factors. That is, despite differences in input, the L2
learner will follow the same developmental path, because, although there are
differences in the types of input to be found in each setting, there are also
similarities.
The natural sequence is the product
of one type language use-spontaneous communication-which, although restricted
in classroom context, does take place. The first explanation follows a native’s
interpretation. What is the quite clear, whatever interpretation is adopted, is
that SLA possesses certain structural properties which are immune to
environmental differences in learnt in classroom and natural setting. The
effect of environmental factors appears to be restricted largely to how quickly
and how much of the L2 the learner acquires.
Language pedagogy
Looking at instruction from the view
point of the learner rather than the teacher is salutary. It puts into
perspective the widely held view that if instruction is based on a sound
syllabus and employs motivating techniques, acquisition will result. Unless
account is taken of the structural properties of SLA, success is by no means
certain.
Teacher ought not to feel obligated
to ensure that his teaching also follows it, as it is far more important that
the teacher works from a syllabus which he finds logically acceptable. Brumfit
argues that language teaching will be most successful when it follows as
well-worked out plan which directs and organizes what the teacher does. The second
reason for reticence is that, although there is a fair degree of agreement
among SLA researches concerning what happens in SLA.
There is a far less agreement about
why it happens in the why it does. This has been evident in the different
positions adopted to explain the result of research into the effects of formal
instruction. Briefly outline what attitude to the code-communication dilemma is
held by protagonists of each of the three positions considered in the previous
section
- The non-interface position.
Krashen (1982) pays close attention
to the role of grammar teaching in classroom SLA. He sees two uses for it.
First, it enables the monitor to function by providing for ‘learning’. However,
monitor use is restricted to occasions when the learner has time to access his
‘learnt’ knowledge, and is also restricted by the fact that only a small
sub-section of total rules of a L2 are ‘learnable’. The second use of grammar
teaching is to satisfy learners’ curiosity about the nature of the L2 grammatical
system ‘grammar appreciation’. The use of conscious grammar is limited,
therefore, that the role of teaching is to afford opportunities for
communications, rather than to draw attention to the L2 code.
Krashen (1981b) lists the defining
characteristic of what he considers an effective pedagogical programme;
- The classroom input must be comprehensible.
- The programme must consist of ‘communicative activities’, as only these will ensure that he input is interesting and relevant.
- There should be no attempt to follow a grammatically sequenced programme.
- The input must be of sufficient quantity (hence importance of extensive reading).
- The interface position
Where the non-interface position
emphasizes the importance of communication and minimizes the importance of the
code, the interface position asserts the contribution of the code.
Sharewoos-Smith (1981) sees grammar teaching as a short cut to communicative
ability. That is, the adult learner who has his attention drawn to features of
the code can practise these, both in and out of the classroom, until he can use
them subconsciously in fluent communicative speech. Consciousness-raising,
therefore, does not require that the learner is able to verbalize the rules he
has learnt. For Sharwood-Smith, then, the important issue is not whether the
code should be taught, but in what way it should be taught.
- The variability position
The variability position stresses
importance of matching the learning process with the type of instruction.
Instruction must consider the specific goals of the learner and attempt to
provide the appropriate form of knowledge to achieve those goals. The ‘goals’
refer to the type of language use that the learner needs (or wants) to engage
in. if the goal is to participate in natural conversation, the learner will
need to develop his vernacular style by acquiring L2 knowledge that is
automatic but unanalysed.
This can be achieved directly by
means of instruction that emphasizes communication in the classroom. It may
also be achieved indirectly by teaching that focuses on the code, if there are
also sufficient practice opportunities to trigger the passage of knowledge from
the careful to the vernacular style.
0 komentar:
Speak up your mind
Tell us what you're thinking... !